Aesthetic Perception of Stage Setups in Dance
##plugins.themes.bootstrap3.article.main##
Dance as moving art is an adequate medium for exploring visual perception and the aesthetic value of movements. The aesthetic experience in dance movements and performances was investigated over the last two decades. Still, research on stage setups in dance is severely underrepresented despite their importance in dance choreographies. The study aimed to assess dancers (hip-hop and modern dance) and non-dancers' aesthetic evaluation of three different prototypical movements performed on five prototypical stage setups. N=27 dancers (hip-hop and modern) and non-dancers evaluated various movements performed on different stage setups on their perceived aesthetic. It was hypothesized that symmetrical stage setups and a fast movement or a movement with a wide form were generally preferred. It was furthermore expected that dancers and non-dancers, as well as dancers from different styles, differ in their aesthetic perception of stage setups and dance movements. Results revealed that the movement contract-release and the stage setup V were generally evaluated as most aesthetic. Nevertheless, while hip-hop dancers and non-dancers preferred a free stage setup as the least aesthetic, modern dancers preferred a bloc setup as the least aesthetic. It can be concluded that there is a general preference for movements comprising a large amplitude and range of motions and for stage setups that contain symmetry and a wide form. Thus, symmetry seems essential when developing stage setups in dance. This can be used as a tool trying to delight the observer as well as trying to play with contrast and convergence throughout a whole dance performance.
References
-
Amir, O., Biederman, I., & Hayworth, K. J. (2011). The neural basis for shape preferences. Vision research, 51(20), 2198-2206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2011.08.015.
Google Scholar
1
-
Berlyne, D. E. (1974). Konflikt, Erregung, Neugier. [Conflict, arousal and curiosity]. Klett.
Google Scholar
2
-
Blake, R., & Shiffrar, M. (2007). Perception of human motion. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 47-73. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190152.
Google Scholar
3
-
Brown D. D.; Wijffels G. & Meulenbroek R.G. J. (2021). Individual Differences in Sequential Movement Coordination in Hip-Hop Dance: Capturing Joint Articulation in Practicing the Wave. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 731901. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.731901.
Google Scholar
4
-
Calvo-Merino, B., Ehrenberg, S., Leung, D., & Haggard, P. (2010). Experts see it all: configural effects in action observation. Psychological Research, 74(4), 400-406. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-009-0262-y.
Google Scholar
5
-
Cupchik, G. C., & Winston, A. S. (1996). Confluence and divergence in empirical aesthetics, philosophy, and mainstream psychology. Handbook of perception and cognition: Cognitive ecology, 61-85.
Google Scholar
6
-
Cross, E. S., & Ticini, L. F. (2012). Neuroaesthetics and beyond: New horizons in applying the science of the brain to the art of dance. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 11(1), 5-16. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-010-9190-y.
Google Scholar
7
-
Deutscher Tanzlehrer- & HipHop-Tanzlehrer Organisation, DTHO. (2021). Turnierordnung der DTHO. German. https://dtho.de/images/Downloads/DTHO_Turnierordnung_01_02_2021.pdf.
Google Scholar
8
-
Deutscher Tanzsport Verband, DTV. (2016). Ratgeber Jazz und Modern Dance. German. https://www.tanzsport.de/files/tanzsport/downloads/sportwelt/jmd/jmd_ratgeber.pdf.
Google Scholar
9
-
Gerber, A. & Mattis, C. (2017). Bewegung inszenieren. [Staging Movement]. Printcenter Berlin.
Google Scholar
10
-
Gilmartin, P. P. (1983). Aesthetic preferences for the proportions and forms of graticules. The Cartographic Journal, 20(2), 95-100. https://doi.org/10.1179/caj.1983.20.2.95.
Google Scholar
11
-
Glass, R. (2005). Observer response to contemporary dance. In R. Grove, C. Stevens & S. McKechnie (Eds.), Thinking in four dimensions: Creativity and cognition in contemporary dance (pp. 107–121). Melbourne University Press.
Google Scholar
12
-
Jacobsen, T., & Höfel, L. (2002). Aesthetic Judgments of Novel Graphic Patterns: Analyses of Individual Judgments. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 95(3), 755–766. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.2002.95.3.755.
Google Scholar
13
-
Jäger, J., & Kuckhermann, R. (2004). Aesthetics and Social Work. Aesthetic Practice in Social Work. Weinheim.
Google Scholar
14
-
Kirsch, L. P., Drommelschmidt, K. A., & Cross, E. S. (2013). The impact of sensorimotor experience on affective evaluation of dance. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 7, 521. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00521.
Google Scholar
15
-
Knudson, D. (2009). Significant and meaningful effects in sports biomechanics research. Sports Biomechanics, 8(1), 96-104. https://doi.org/10.1080/14763140802629966.
Google Scholar
16
-
Latto, R., Brain, D., & Kelly, B. (2000). An Oblique Effect in Aesthetics: Homage to Mondrian (1872–1944). Perception, 29(8), 981–987. https://doi.org/10.1068/p2352.
Google Scholar
17
-
Latto, R., & Russell-Duff, K. (2002). An Oblique Effect in the Selection of Line Orientation by Twentieth Century Painters. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 20(1), 49–60. https://doi.org/10.2190/3VEY-RC3B-9GM7-KGDY.
Google Scholar
18
-
Makin, A. D., Helmy, M., & Bertamini, M. (2018). Visual cortex activation predicts visual preference: evidence from Britain and Egypt. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(8), 1771-1780. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2017.1350870.
Google Scholar
19
-
Marković, S. (2010). Aesthetic experience and the emotional content of paintings. Psihologija, 43(1), 47-64. https://doi.org/10.2298/PSI1001047M.
Google Scholar
20
-
Marković, S. (2012). Components of aesthetic experience: aesthetic fascination, aesthetic appraisal, and aesthetic emotion. i-Perception, 3(1), 1-17. https://doi.org/10.1068/i0450aap.
Google Scholar
21
-
Martindale, C., & Moore, K. (1988). Priming, prototypicality, and preference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(4), 661–670. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.14.4.661.
Google Scholar
22
-
Orgs, G., Hagura, N., & Haggard, P. (2013). Learning to like it: aesthetic perception of bodies, movements and choreographic structure. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(2), 603-612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.03.010.
Google Scholar
23
-
Orlandi, A., Cross, E. S., & Orgs, G. (2020). Timing is everything: Dance aesthetics depend on the complexity of movement kinematics. Cognition, 205, 104446. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104446.
Google Scholar
24
-
Palmer, S. E., & Griscom, W. S. (2013). Accounting for taste: Individual differences in preference for harmony. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 20(3), 453-461. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0355-2.
Google Scholar
25
-
Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing Fluency and Aesthetic Pleasure: Is Beauty in the Perceiver’s Processing Experience? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(4), 364–382. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_3.
Google Scholar
26
-
Rosenberg, C. (2000). Handbuch Jazz dance. Meyer & Meyer sports. German.
Google Scholar
27
-
Sato, N., Nunome, H., & Ikegami, Y. (2014). Key features of hip hop dance motions affect evaluation by judges. Journal of applied biomechanics, 30(3), 439-445. https://doi.org/10.1123/jab.2013-0190.
Google Scholar
28
-
Sato, N., & Nunome, H. (2016). Key motion characteristics of side-step movements in hip-hop dance and their effect on the evaluation by judges. Sports Biomechanics, 15(2), 116-127. https://doi.org/10.1080/14763141.2016.1158861.
Google Scholar
29
-
Silvia, P. J., & Barona, C. M. (2009). Do People Prefer Curved Objects? Angularity, Expertise, and Aesthetic Preference. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 27(1), 25–42. https://doi.org/10.2190/EM.27.1.b.
Google Scholar
30
-
Song, J., Kwak, Y., & Kim, C. Y. (2021). Familiarity and Novelty in Aesthetic Preference: The Effects of the Properties of the Artwork and the Beholder. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 1-17. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.694927.
Google Scholar
31
-
Stevens, C.; Glass, R.; Schubert, E.; Chen, J. & Winskel, H. (2007). Methods for Measuring Audience Reactions. Proceedings of the inaugural International Conference on Music Communication Science, 155-158.
Google Scholar
32
-
Tinio, P. P., & Leder, H. (2009). Just how stable are stable aesthetic features? Symmetry, complexity, and the jaws of massive familiarization. Acta psychologica, 130(3), 241-250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.01.001.
Google Scholar
33
-
Torrents, C., Castañer, M., Jofre, T., Morey, G., & Reverter, F. (2013). Kinematic parameters that influence the aesthetic perception of beauty in contemporary dance. Perception, 42, 447–458. https://doi.org/10.1068/p7117.
Google Scholar
34
-
Vinken, P. M. (2022). Kinematic motion characteristics and observer’s expertise in perceived aesthetics of dance jumps. Research in Dance Education, 1-17. https://doi.org/10.1080/14647893.2022.2033714.
Google Scholar
35
-
Vinken, P.M. & Heinen T. (2022). How does the amount of movement and observer expertise shape the perception of motion aesthetics in dance? Human Movement, 23(2), 46–55; https://doi.org/10.5114/hm.2021.106170.
Google Scholar
36
-
Wessel-Therhorn, D. (1996). Jazz dance training. Meyer and Meyer. German.
Google Scholar
37
-
Winkelman, P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Mind at ease puts a smile on the face: Psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation elicits positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 989–1000. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.989.
Google Scholar
38
-
Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025848.
Google Scholar
39
Most read articles by the same author(s)
-
Frederike Veit,
Juliane Veit,
Thomas Heinen,
The Influence of Music on Judges’ Evaluation of Complex Skills in Gymnastics , European Journal of Sport Sciences: Vol. 1 No. 5 (2022)